Camouflage is the use of any combination of materials, coloration, or illumination for concealment, either by making animals or objects hard to see (crypsis), or by disguising them as something else (mimesis).
Peacock Flounder: it can change according to the environment |
History:
The study of camouflage has a long history in biology, and the numerous ways of concealment and disguise found in the animal kingdom provided Darwin and Wallace with important examples for illustrating and defending their ideas of natural selection and adaptation. Thus, various forms
of camouflage have become classic examples of evolution. In a broader sense, camouflage has been adopted by humans, most notably by the military and hunters, but it has also influenced other parts of society, for example, arts, popular culture, and design.
Grasshopper: Professional in disguise |
Animals use camouflage to make detection or recognition more difficult, with most examples associated with visual
camouflage involving body coloration. However, in addition to coloration, camouflage may make use of morphological structures or material found in the environment, and may even act against senses other than vision (Ruxton 2009).
Dead Leaf Butterfly: the perfect camouflage |
In nature, some of the most striking examples of adaptation can be found with respect to avoid being detected or recognized, with the strategies employed diverse and sometimes extraordinary. Such strategies
can include using markings to match the color and pattern of the background,
as in various moths (e.g. Kettlewell 1955), and to break up the appearance of
the body, as in some marine isopods (Merilaita 1998).
Camouflage is a technique
especially useful if the animal can change color to match the background on
which it is found, such as can some cephalopods (Hanlon & Messenger 1988)
and chameleons. Further remarkable examples include
insects bearing an uncanny resemblance to bird droppings or fish resembling fallen leaves on a stream bed (Sazima et al. 2006), to even making the body effectively transparent, as occurs in a range of, in particular, aquatic species.
suggest that prey may instead choose more
complex visual backgrounds since background complexity is known to impede
visual detection of targets. The authors compared choice between differently patterned achromatic backgrounds,
controlled to have the same amount of black and white across treatments, but
with different arrangements and orientations of stripes and other small shapes.
The fish
generally preferred backgrounds that allowed better matching with the orientation and shape of their body markings, although in one instance females preferred
a more complex background.
(c) Individual-level choices
Variation in appearance within species is frequently not characterized by discrete morphs, but rather by continuous individual variation (although it can be challenging to distinguish the two). Many species show considerable variation among individuals, and this can vary with age and ability to change color. The ability of individuals to select backgrounds that match their own unique appearance has not been widely considered until recently, except to an extent in color-changing species, but several studies have recently tested this idea. One of the most comprehensive studies of individual background choice was on Japanese quail
(Coturnix japonica) (Lovell et al.,2013). Quail lay eggs that
are highly divergent in appearance among
females; some mothers lay light eggs with little maculation, whereas others lay
dark eggs with a prominence of dark markings.
In lizards, background choice seems linked to both individual appearance and predation risk. Marshall, Philpot & Stevens (2016) showed that in Aegean wall lizards (Podarcis erhardii),
individuals were found on chosen backgrounds that resembled their own individual dorsal coloration (to predator vision models) better than would be the case if found in locations chosen by other individuals.
Webster et al. (2009) found evidence that some species of Catocala moth have consistent orientation behavior, whereas other species of moth do not. They presented human subjects with images of moths superimposed on images of trees with changed orientation, and found that orientation was a key
attribute contributing to detection. In
addition, when they rotated the tree bark images horizontally, the optimal
orientation changed accordingly, showing that the effect of orientation on
detection was not due to the moth position per se, but rather its
interaction with background features.
Animal
camouflage represents one of the most important ways of preventing (or
facilitating) predation. It attracted the attention of the earliest
evolutionary biologists and today remains a focus of investigation in areas
ranging from evolutionary ecology, animal decision-making, optimal strategies,
visual psychology, computer science, to materials science. Most work focuses on
the role of animal morphology per se and its interactions with the background
in affecting detection and recognition. However, the behavior of organisms is
likely to be crucial in affecting camouflage too, through
- background choice
- body orientation
- positioning
- strategies of camouflage that require movement.
In most natural
environments, animals face a problem in ensuring that their camouflage is
effective – most visual environments vary. This means that a single fixed
phenotype is unlikely to be optimally concealed against all or even many
potential backgrounds. Three main solutions exist.
- Many animals change color for camouflage, enabling them to tune their appearance to the background. However, while a few animals such as cephalopods, chameleons, and some fish can rapidly adjust their appearance in seconds, color change in most animals take longer, meaning that there will often be a mismatch between appearance and background during changes.
- Animals might adopt a ‘compromise’ appearance, which matches no background perfectly but several to some degree although evidence for how widespread this approach is in nature is lacking.
- For animals to choose where to rest or sit in a way that best matches their appearance.
The most obvious
way that animals could use behavior to improve camouflage is through choosing
to rest on backgrounds that match their own appearance. This
could arise at a number of levels.
- First, all individuals of a species may have the same fixed preference for a background type (e.g. all individuals always show a preference for black backgrounds; species-level choice).
- Second, all individuals may show the same preferences, but these are context-dependent (flexible species-level choices). For example, when Inhabitat a, all individuals choose white, but all choose black in habitat b. Context factors include not just habitat but also, for example, age, activity, reproductive status, or level of parasitic infection.
Evidence of
background choice
(a) Species-level choices
(a) Species-level choices
Boardmanet
al. (1974) undertook tests with more natural backgrounds, finding that those species
which normally preferred black or white in uniform controlled apparatus also
tended to choose dark or light natural substrates, respectively. There was general consistency in the coloration and pattern types of individuals and the
substrates that they chose. For example, grey and mottled individuals tended to
choose sandy grey soils, whereas individuals with green markings tended to use
greener leaves. In other work, recently metamorphosed American toads (Bufo
americanus) show preferences for dark soil and mixed sandy substrates
over
plain sand and this coincides with a higher predation risk on plain sand
backgrounds from snakes (Heinen, 1993). Some lizards have also been shown to
prefer microhabitats that are likely to confer better camouflage. Studies
manipulating aspects like vegetation cover also report that ground-nesting
birds choose backgrounds related to camouflage and
American toad: Bufo americanus |
Common potoo (Nyctibius griseus) |
predation risk. In species like the common potoo (Nyctibius griseus), which
subjectively often ensemble branches of trees (masquerade), choice of perch sites
seems driven for both enhanced background matching and masquerade. Instead of choosing backgrounds that individuals' best match, the authors
(b)
Morph-specific choices
Many species
exist in a number of discrete morphs, and here we would expect background
choice to be consistent with morph appearance. Kettlewell (1955) first tested
this in an experiment with pale (typical) and melanic (carbonara) forms of the
peppered moth (Biston betularia) and found support
more likely to be
attacked than camouflaged individuals. This contrasts with previous experiments
on the same color morphs (Brattstrom & Warren, 1955) that found no
evidence of substrate choice. In behavioral choice experiments (Wente &
Phillips, 2005), fixed green frogs preferred matching substrates, but brown
morphs only preferred matching substrates when in the presence of predator
(snake odor) cues.
Peppered moth (Biston betularia) |
for each morph
choosing appropriate dark or light backgrounds. Furthermore, in predation
experiments with garter snakes (Thamnophis elegans), mismatched frogs were
(c) Individual-level choices
Variation in appearance within species is frequently not characterized by discrete morphs, but rather by continuous individual variation (although it can be challenging to distinguish the two). Many species show considerable variation among individuals, and this can vary with age and ability to change color. The ability of individuals to select backgrounds that match their own unique appearance has not been widely considered until recently, except to an extent in color-changing species, but several studies have recently tested this idea. One of the most comprehensive studies of individual background choice was on Japanese quail
In lizards, background choice seems linked to both individual appearance and predation risk. Marshall, Philpot & Stevens (2016) showed that in Aegean wall lizards (Podarcis erhardii),
individuals were found on chosen backgrounds that resembled their own individual dorsal coloration (to predator vision models) better than would be the case if found in locations chosen by other individuals.
(d)
Color-changing species
Differences in
individual appearance frequently arises due to the ability of some animals to
change color (Duarte et al., 2017a). Here, we would expect that individuals of
species that can change color fairly slowly show background choice (with
choice changing to maximize camouflage as the animal changes color). By
contrast, in animals with rapid color change there may be no need for
substrate choice since they can match many backgrounds quickly (subject to constraints
regarding the degree of match possible). The available evidence broadly supports this prediction, but not for all
species or individuals. For example, work on cuttlefish (Sepia
Officinalis) has tended not to reveal strong preferences for substrate appearance (Allen et al., 2010), although one study on Sepia pharaonic(Lee, Yan & Chiao, 2012) found that cuttlefish raised in an environment with enriched visual information including light and dark rocks and artificial algae showed greater/earlier preferences for high-contrast patterned backgrounds than individuals reared in environments of plain grey or checkerboard patterns.
(2) The ecological context of choice
Officinalis) has tended not to reveal strong preferences for substrate appearance (Allen et al., 2010), although one study on Sepia pharaonic(Lee, Yan & Chiao, 2012) found that cuttlefish raised in an environment with enriched visual information including light and dark rocks and artificial algae showed greater/earlier preferences for high-contrast patterned backgrounds than individuals reared in environments of plain grey or checkerboard patterns.
(2) The ecological context of choice
Background-choice the behaviour of animals should have a range of important outcomes and implications
for the exploitation of resources and (micro-)habitats. In the first instance, a
range of camouflaged animals shows phenotype–environment associations, which can
lead to matching to specific habitats (Stevens et al., 2015; Xiao et
al., 2016). Such associations and matching can arise through genetic
adaptation (e.g. Rosenblum et al., 2010) or phenotypic plasticity and
ontogenetic color change (Todd et al., 2006; Stevens, Lown & Wood,
2014). However, even in phenotypically plastic species, such as shore crabs (Carcinus
maenas), which are extremely variable in color and pattern, background
choice is also likely to explain associations at the microscale.Behavior and camouflage type can also be linked to habitat use in other ways. For example, the prawn Hippolyte obliquimanus exists in two main morphs – the homogenous type that
uses background
matching to resemble different algal backgrounds, and a striped form that seems to rely on
transparency for concealment (Duarte & Flores, 2017). The homogenous types are able to change color between
algal background species, and stick closely to their matching substrate,
whereas the transparent the form shows less background affinity and more
mobile behavior and associated morphology.
ORIENTATION, POSTURE, SHAPE, AND HIDING
SHADOWS
Behavior can be used to fine-tune
camouflage by controlling the orientation, posture, and shape of the organism.
(1) Orientation and positioning behavior
Webster et al. (2009) found evidence that some species of Catocala moth have consistent orientation behavior, whereas other species of moth do not. They presented human subjects with images of moths superimposed on images of trees with changed orientation, and found that orientation was a key
(2) Posture, shape, and minimizing shadows
The positions that animals adopt can also affect types of camouflage beyond crypsis, such as masquerade. Suzuki & Sakurai (2015) noted that many caterpillars which are thought to
resemble bird droppings rest in a posture with their bodies curled up or bent,
seemingly increasing their similarity to real droppings. They created an
artificial caterpillar prey that was either black and white or green
and pinned them either in a straight posture or bent, and measured predation
from birds in the field. Models that were green (cryptic) did not differ in
survival with posture, whereas black-and-white models resembling bird droppings
survived better when placed in a bent posture.
MECHANISMS OF CHOICE AND ORIENTATION
Here we focus both on the
senses that animals use to control their behavior and the underlying mechanisms
that govern how information from the senses is used to control behavior:
whether fixed genetic preference, imprinting, assessment of own camouflage, or
otherwise.
(1) Use of different sensory modalities to judge the background
A range of sensory information can be used to find suitable backgrounds
and resting sites. For example, some shrimp use visual information on shape,
size, and contrast to locate
preferred habitats (Barry, 1974). Gillis (1982)
studied substrate choice in the polymorphic grasshopper Circotettix rabula and found that when the eyes of individual grasshoppers were
damaged, substrate choice disappeared.
(2) How
animals could choose correct backgrounds
One of the most important issues regarding background choice and positioning concerns the
mechanisms that enable animals to make appropriate choices. There are several
potential ways this could be achieved.
- First, there may exist a genetic basis for choice (‘preference gene’), linked to genes governing appearance.
- Second, animals could imprint on or learn about certain visual backgrounds that are important or that they are likely to associate with.
- Third, individuals may actively use their senses to assess how closely their body coloration matches the backgrounds that they choose.
Animals
interact behaviourally with the world around them, and so now we focus on
aspects of this interaction related to the exploitation of materials from the
environment (or secreted by the animal itself) to influence their camouflage.
(1) Decoration
Ruxton & Stevens (2015, p. 2) reviewed the literature on
decorating by animals, and define a decorator as ‘an organism that (by means of
specialist behavior and/or morphology that has been favored by selection for
that purpose) accumulates and retains environmental material that becomes
attached to the exterior of the decorator’.
(2) Modifying the visual background
We have already considered how animals
can influence camouflage behaviourally by a selection of the background against
which they are seen. It seems at least conceptually n plausible that animals
could go a step further and behaviourally modify that environment to enhance
crypsis.
(3) Hiding built structures
The evidence that animals employ the behavior to hide the structures that they build is scarce. Almost by
definition, traps such as spider’s webs must be constructed in such a way as to
be difficult for prey to detect in order to be effective, and there is evidence
that certain specific features of webs can be linked to reduced avoidance by
prey. Turning to homes built
by animals,
Bailey et al. (2014) provide evidence that birds may actively select
materials that camouflage their nests. They demonstrate that captive zebra
finches (Taeniopygia guttata) preferentially select nesting material
that is similar in color to the provided nest cup and surrounding cage walls.
MOVEMENT
It is generally considered that stillness
is an integral aspect to camouflage, and there are many reports of cryptic prey
‘freezing’ in response to heightened danger from visual predators (e.g. Caro,
2005). This is backed up by experimental demonstrations that movement by an organism with cryptic color and patterning can greatly increase its chance of
detection (e.g. Ioannou & Krause, 2009; Stevens et al., 2011). Here
we explore if there might be some exceptions to this generality.
(1) Flicker-fusion
The flicker-fusion effect occurs if a patterned object moves sufficiently quickly across the visual field of a viewer that the
patterning becomes blurred and the appearance of the patterned object changes
(Endler, 1978; Stevens, 2007; Umeton, Read & Rowe, 2017). It is plausible
that such an effect could reduce the predation risk of moving objects by providing
a better match to their background, but while this has been postulated to occur
in some fast-moving striped snakes, the evidence is scant.
(2) Motion dazzle
Motion dazzle is the phenomenon where
the pattern of a moving object can make an estimation of its speed and/or trajectory
harder (Thayer, 1909; Stevens, 2007; Stevens, Yule & Ruxton, 2008). Proof
of concept in artificial systems has been repeatedly demonstrated for human
observers (e.g. Scott-Samuel et al., 2011; Stevens et al., 2011, 2008,
and references therein), but highly suggestive evidence also exists
for
non-human observers.
Patterning that may cause a motion-dazzle effect has been hypothesized for a
number of animals (especially lizards: Halperin, Carmel & Hawlena, 2017;
Murali & Kodandaramaiah, 2018), but has been investigated most fully in the
case of zebra (Equus spp.) stripes.
(3) Motion to facilitate masquerade
Bian, Elgar & Peters (2016)
present evidence that is suggestive that a stick insect (Extatsoma tiaratum)
shows swaying behavior in windy conditions in order to reduce the ease with
which it can be distinguished visually from surrounding moving foliage. They
found that the frequencies
adopted by the insect overlapped strongly with those
of the surrounding foliage. Further, they show that this is an environmentally sensitive
behavior – with swaying being more sustained in response to time-varying
(blustery) wind and ceasing at high wind-speeds.
ROLE OF BEHAVIOURAL CAMOUFLAGE IN
A CHANGING WORLD
Humans are having a huge impact on
other organisms globally, but these impacts are diverse and not always easy to
predict. Here we provide case studies that argue for behaviourally mediated
camouflage to be considered an important trait that can mediate the impact of
humans on different animals in nature.
Finally, it is possible that
behavioral flexibility in camouflage may sometimes be an important trait
facilitating the invasion of natural habitats consequent to deliberate or inadvertent
introduction by humans. The shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is known as a
globally invasive species (Darling et al., 2008), and several studies
have shown phenotype–environment associations.
Camouflage has long been known to be mediated by behavior, including potentially in the choice
of appropriate resting backgrounds, body
positions and orientations, hiding key features
such as shadows, maintaining concealment during
motion, and modifying bodies, structures, and surroundings.